Highlights

Print Post
  • All by itself, a higher AFQT score produces marked average gains in income. Tweet This
  • Intelligence affects income both directly and by virtue of its indirect effect through education (i.e., more intelligent people are more likely to attend college, so they ultimately make more money). Tweet This

Author’s NoteThis is a revised version of a post that originally appeared on my personal blog. It can be found here. It includes additional methodological details for the statistically curious.

I sometimes encounter people I’ve come to think of as “IQ truthers.”  IQ is the magic social science variable that explains virtually every observed correlation. Racial differences? The intergenerational transmission of wealth? The effects of parenting or family structure? All of it must boil down to differences in IQ.

As a social scientist, I’m naturally resistant to single-variable theories. The world is just too complex for that. But it’s much more persuasive for me to respond with data. I was motivated to do so when I realized I had the appropriate national data at hand: the 1979 cohort of the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth (NLSY-79). This is a representative sample of over 10,000 Americans between the ages of 14 and 22 initially interviewed in 1979, then annually or biennially thereafter. My subset ends in 2014. My NLSY-79 research looks at the economics of motherhood, so my analytic sample is limited to men and women with kids (hey, this is a blog post, not a peer-reviewed article). This leaves me with a sample size of about 7,100, more than enough for population estimates.

Early on in the panel, respondents were administered a variety of IQ tests, as well as the similar Armed Forces Qualifying Test. Only about 1,700 respondents got the IQ tests, while all were given the AFQT. AFQT and IQ measure essentially the same things and are highly correlated (r = .78). Accordingly, I’ll report results based on AFQT scores. Analysis based on IQ produced essentially the same results.

My existing research looks at family income, so I’ll go with it as the dependent variable here. It’s a better measure of living standards than individual income is. It’s top-coded at $300,000, logged for all regression analyses, and converted into 2018 dollars to account for inflation.

Do higher AFQT scores predict a higher income? Of course they do, as this graphic shows:


Note: This is a lowess regression model. The dependent variable is the median annual family income for all 26 waves of NLSY data.

But a lot of other things also predict income. So, what’s the unique contribution of AFQT scores? More precisely, how much of the variability in income can they explain?  Statisticians often answer this question by reporting a statistic called r-squared that varies from zero to one. In this analysis, zero means AFQT has no predictive power, while one would mean that someone’s income can be perfectly predicted by knowing their AFQT score. An r-squared of .5 would mean that half of the variation in income could be explained by knowing someone’s AFQT scores (or, less scientifically, half the time you can predict someone’s income by knowing how they did on the AFQT).

The data show that AFQT scores explain 21% of the variation in income between survey respondents. That translates to a correlation coefficient of .46. 

Is that a large correlation? It depends upon your perspective. If your cup is half full, you can correctly point out that .46 rivals the largest observed correlations in social psychology, sociology, and other relevant fields. But if your cup is half empty, you’ll say that many things determine how much money people make, and smarts is only one of them.

In fact, the true contribution of AFQT to income is probably smaller. That’s because AFQT is serving as a proxy for other attributes correlated with earnings. People with high AFQT scores probably stayed in school longer, and most likely had more successful parents. These and other correlates of intelligence factor into the aforementioned 21 percent.

To look at this, I estimated additional regression models.1 One of them included a bevy of independent variables, all things social scientists generally use to predict income. Many of these attributes are measured at each survey wave: marital status and presence of a live-in partner, education, number of children, employment status, employment history, hours worked, and age. Measures of race/ethnicity and sex are also included. I compare this to a model including only income. This lets me determine how much the predictive power of intelligence is reduced after accounting for basic social and demographic differences between respondents.

All by itself, a higher AFQT score produces marked average gains in income, as the orange trend line in the figure above shows. The lowest scoring NLSY respondents make an average of $20,000, while the most intelligent earn almost six figures. But a good chunk of that difference goes away after accounting for demographic differences between respondents, as the blue line shows.

Some caveats. My data are nationally representative, but it’s a sample of parents, not all Americans. Childless Americans are excluded. Most of the respondents are now in their 50s. Perhaps the influence of intelligence on earnings has changed over time. I’m also looking at family income rather than individual income. That having been said, I don’t expect it to matter much. More intelligent people generally make more money; when married, their spouses make more money. The differences will wash out in a large sample like mine.

Several readers of my original post suggested that there are endogeneity problems with the analysis used to predict the blue trend line. In plain English: education, marital status, and the like are all downstream of income. AFQT might influence income directly, or indirectly (for example, smarter people are more likely to stay in school, which in turn affects income).

Can this proposition be tested empirically? It can—if we’re willing to make some assumptions. For instance, if we reasonably assume AFQT is anterior to education and education is anterior to income, we can explore AFQT’s direct and indirect effects—as mediated by education—on income via path analysis (the people who want latent variables in their path analysis are asking for a better measurement of IQ than is available in my data). I also have nothing to offer to anyone seeking unicorns of model identification.

I’ll limit the demonstration to the role of education in explaining the effects of IQ on income. The point of this blog post is to show that intelligence is just one of many variables that matter in explaining the world; it’s not to conduct an exhaustive exploration of the predictors of income, nor to identify the extent to which all measured variables at my disposal can mediate the effects of intelligence. Education is a good place to start: it has real-world significance and a noteworthy correlation with earnings. The conceptual model is shown in Figure 3.

Figure 3. How AFQT affects income as mediated by education

Here are the results based on standardized regression coefficients.2 The total effect of AFQT on income (.505) can be divided into partial (.265) and indirect (.237) effects (in a world without rounding error, .265 and .237 would sum to .505, the zero-order effect). The partial effect of AFQT (.265) reflects the extent to which it affects income, net of differences in educational attainment. The indirect effect (.237) represents the extent to which AFQT increases income by increasing education. The partial effect of education (.345) reflects the extent to which education affects income net of AFQT. It’s not rocket surgery to observe that both AFQT and education have large partial effects on income. Indeed, the partial effect of education (.345) is larger than that of AFQT (.265), although the overall effect of AFQT is larger: intelligence affects income both directly and by virtue of its indirect effect through education (i.e., more intelligent people are more likely to attend college, so they ultimately make more money).

Does IQ affect income and other measures of human accomplishment? It does—but so, too, do a lot of other things. The foregoing results suggest intelligence is not the cardinal social science variable, or to paraphrase Tolkien: It’s not the one variable to rule them all, one variable to find them, one variable to bring them all and in the darkness bind them.

Nicholas H. Wolfinger is Professor of Family and Consumer Studies and Adjunct Professor of Sociology at the University of Utah. His most recent book is Soul Mates: Religion, Sex, Children, and Marriage among African Americans and Latinos, coauthored with W. Bradford Wilcox (Oxford University Press, 2016). Follow him on Twitter at @NickWolfinger.


1. I estimate random-effects regression models using the 26 waves of NLSY79 data at my disposal. Hausman tests suggest that fixed-effects fit the data better, but don’t allow me to explore fundamentally time-invariant predictors like AFQT scores (in any event, they were only administered once to respondents). My dependent variable is the natural logarithm of inflation-adjusted family income. In addition, I repeated my analyses using OLS regression to predict log-median income across survey waves and obtained similar results.

2. The results of the regressions relevant to the path analysis look like this:

Note: These estimates were obtained via OLS, as described above. The dependent variable is median log-income. I converted education into a continuous variable to facilitate interpretation. All effects are statistically significant (p < .001) based on Huber-White standard errors. I’m generally opposed to standardized coefficients (the βs), but they’re useful here.